The Human Memory - what it is, how it works and how it can go wrong
Home
Contact
Search
The Human Memory - what it is, how it works and how it can go wrong
INTRODUCTION
TYPES OF MEMORY
MEMORY PROCESSES
MEMORY DISORDERS
MEMORY & THE BRAIN
SOURCES & REFERENCES

Memory Processes
  Introduction
  Memory Encoding
  Memory Consolidation
  Memory Storage
  Memory Recall/Retrieval


MEMORY STORAGE

??? Did You Know ???
Genuine eidetic or photographic memory (an “unprocessed” sensory memory of sensory events that is as accurate as if the person were still experiencing the original objects or events) is extremely rare, although not unheard of.
Most extraordinary memory skills which make claims of photographic memory, however, result from a combination of innate skills, learned tactics, mnemonic devices and extraordinary knowledge bases, rather than eidetic memory as such.
Young children, with their lack of verbal and conceptual systems, are more likely to have real eidetic memories than adults.
A classic case from the 1970s was a woman named Elizabeth, who was able to write out poetry in a foreign language (of which she had no prior knowledge) several years after seeing the original text.
Storage is the more or less passive process of retaining information in the brain, whether in the sensory memory, the short-term memory or the more permanent long-term memory. Each of these different stages of human memory function as a sort of filter that helps to protect us from the flood of information that confront us on a daily basis, avoiding an overload of information and helping to keep us sane. The more the information is repeated or used, the more likely it is to be retained in long-term memory (which is why, for example, studying helps people to perform better on tests). This process of consolidation, the stabilizing of a memory trace after its initial acquisition, is treated in more detail in a separate section.

Since the early neurological work of Karl Lashley and Wilder Penfield in the 1950s and 1960s, it has become clear that long-term memories are not stored in just one part of the brain, but are widely distributed throughout the cortex. After consolidation, long-term memories are stored throughout the brain as groups of neurons that are primed to fire together in the same pattern that created the original experience, and each component of a memory is stored in the brain area that initiated it (e.g. groups of neurons in the visual cortex store a sight, neurons in the amygdala store the associated emotion, etc). Indeed, it seems that they may even be encoded redundantly, several times, in various parts of the cortex, so that, if one engram (or memory trace) is wiped out, there are duplicates, or alternative pathways, elsewhere, through which the memory may still be retrieved.

Therefore, contrary to the popular notion, memories are not stored in our brains like books on library shelves, but must be actively reconstructed from elements scattered throughout various areas of the brain by the encoding process. Memory storage is therefore an ongoing process of reclassification resulting from continuous changes in our neural pathways, and parallel processing of information in our brains.

The indications are that, in the absence of disorders due to trauma or neurological disease, the human brain has the capacity to store almost unlimited amounts of information indefinitely. Forgetting, therefore, is more likely to be result from incorrectly or incompletely encoded memories, and/or problems with the recall/retrieval process. It is a common experience that we may try to remember something one time and fail, but then remember that same item later. The information is therefore clearly still there in storage, but there may have been some kind of a mismatch between retrieval cues and the original encoding of the information. “Lost” memories recalled with the aid of psychotherapy or hypnosis are other examples supporting this idea, although it is difficult to be sure that such memories are real and not implanted by the treatment.

Having said that, though, it seems unlikely that, as Richard Schiffrin and others have claimed, ALL memories are stored somewhere in the brain, and that it is only in the retrieval process that irrelevant details are “fast-forwarded” over or expurgated. It seems more likely that the memories which are stored are in some way edited and sorted, and that some of the more peripheral details are never stored.

Forgetting, then, is perhaps better thought of as the temporary or permanent inability to retrieve a piece of information or a memory that had previously been recorded in the brain. Forgetting typically follows a logarithmic curve, so that information loss is quite rapid at the start, but becomes slower as time goes on. In particular, information that has been learned very well (e.g. names, facts, foreign-language vocabulary, etc), will usually be very resistant to forgetting, especially after the first three years. Unlike amnesia, forgetting is usually regarded as a normal phenomenon involving specific pieces of content, rather than relatively broad categories of memories or even entire segments of memory.

??? Did You Know ???
Research using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) suggests that verbs and nouns are stored in different ways in the brain.
Concrete nouns are stored in areas of the brain used to sense or manipulate the referent objects, leading to a theory of meaning based largely on function.

Theorists disagree over exactly what becomes of material that is forgotten. Some hold that long-term memories do actually decay and disappear completely over time; others hold that the memory trace remains intact as long as we live, but the bonds or cues that allow us to retrieve the trace become broken, due to changes in the organization of the neural network, new experiences, etc, in the same way as a misplaced book in a library is “lost” even though it still exists somewhere in the library.

Interestingly, it appears not to be possible to deliberately delete memories at will, which can have negative consequences, for example if we experience traumatic events we would actually prefer to forget. In fact, such memories tend to be imprinted even more strongly than normal due to their emotional content, although recent research involving the use of beta blockers (such as propanonol) suggests that it may be possible to tone down the emotional aspects of such memories, even if the memories themselves cannot be erased. The way this works is that the act of recalling stored memories makes them "malleable" once more, as they were during the initial encoding phase, and their re-storage can then be blocked by drugs which inhibit the proteins that enable the emotional memory to be re-saved.

 
Back to Top of Page
Home | Contact | Search
Introduction | Types of Memory | Memory Processes | Memory Disorders | Memory & the Brain | Sources & References
 
© 2010 Luke Mastin
 

what is memory, what is human memory